Skip to main content

Feasibility of using a biofeedback device in mindfulness training - a pilot randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

Stress can negatively impact an individual’s health and well-being and high levels of stress are noted to exist among college students today. While traditional treatment methods are plagued with stigma and transfer problems, newly developed wearable biofeedback devices may offer unexplored possibilities. Although these products are becoming commonplace and inexpensive, scientific evidence of the effectiveness of these products is scarce and their feasibility within research contexts are relatively unexplored. Conversely, companies are not required, and possibly reluctant, to release information on the efficacy of these products against their claims. Thus, in the present pilot, we assess the feasibility of using a real-time respiratory-based biofeedback device in preparation for a larger study. Our main aims were to assess device-adherence and collaboration with the company that develops and sells the device.

Method

Data were collected from 39 college students who self-identified as experiencing chronic stress at a Southwestern university in the USA. Students were randomized into either a mindfulness-only control group without a biofeedback device (n = 21), or an experimental group with biofeedback device (n = 18). Both groups received mindfulness meditation training. Pre-test and post-test procedures were conducted 2 weeks apart. Further, both participant compliance and company compliance were assessed and collaboration with the company was evaluated.

Results

Participant device-adherence as well as the company’s collaboration necessary for a full-scale study was determined to be low. This may also have affected our results which showed a strong main effect for time for all outcome variables, suggesting all groups showed improvement in their levels of stress after the intervention period. No group by time effects were identified, however, indicating no added benefit of the biofeedback device.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest feasibility of future studies requires full collaboration and detailed and agreed upon data sharing procedures with the biofeedback company. The particular device under investigation added no value to the intervention outcomes and it was not feasible to continue a larger-scale study. Further, as the technology sector is innovating faster than it can validate products, we urge for open science collaborations between public and private sectors to properly develop evidence-based regulations that can withstand technological innovation while maintaining product quality, safety, and effectiveness.

Trial registration

NCT02837016. Registered 19 July 2016.

Peer Review reports

Background

Stress and barriers to treatment

Mental health problems related to stress and anxiety are an increasingly prevalent issue facing college students today [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. This is an alarming trend considering the host of negative associations related to stress and anxiety such as decreased academic performance1 [8,9,10], school satisfaction [11], depression [5, 6, 12], health problems [13, 14], substance use [5, 15,16,17,18], poor sleep [19], unhealthy eating behavior [18, 20], and reduced self-efficacy [21, 22]. With this in mind, stress is an important target for prevention and treatment on college campuses. Despite the general effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) [23,24,25,26,27,28], there are problems reducing its ultimate usefulness as a treatment for some people related to a lack of help-seeking behavior due to stigma, time-constraints, privacy concerns, and costs [29,30,31]. Furthermore, a problem that traditional treatment faces is the difficulty individuals experience applying the techniques learned in therapy to real life emotionally charged problems. This has also been dubbed the transfer problem [32]. Notably, stress and anxiety may be particularly susceptible to the transfer problem as patients often receive interventions in calm, controlled settings removed from distressing events or triggers that may lead to stressful thoughts or feelings. Once faced with these stressors, however, they may find it difficult to apply what was learned in therapy in these moments when they lack situational control. Furthermore, the emotions of fear that grip people in stressful states is characterized by reduced cognitive control and a rigid fixation on perceived concerns [33,34,35] making it even more challenging to break loose from habitual and unhelpful thought patterns.

With the above in mind, it is important to consider and develop treatment options that can effectively reduce stress and anxiety while also circumventing the many barriers that reduce the usefulness of traditional treatment related to help-seeking and transfer. The primary aim of the present pilot was to assess the feasibility of testing a low-cost wearable biofeedback device based on mindfulness medication techniques in college students with high levels of self-reported stress.

Meditation, mindfulness, and biofeedback devices as possible solutions

Meditation interventions may address some of the shortcomings of traditional treatment. For instance, an array of resources exists for meditation, including online videos, apps, and tutorials, addressing the cost and privacy barriers that exist with traditional treatments. Additionally, meditation is often associated with a lifestyle and may not carry the stigma of being in therapy which may make it easier for individuals to seek treatment. Furthermore, self-reported improvements in stress levels have been indicated in as little as 30 min [36, 37] reducing the time and commitment needed in order for individuals to feel and see improvement. Notably, evidence for the effectiveness of meditation is increasing. Meditation based practices and trainings have been shown to improve individual’s abilities to regulate attention [38], improve executive functioning [38, 39], improve emotion regulation [40], and strengthen neural systems needed for emotion regulation [41]. In relation to stress, a number of studies have found that regular mindfulness practices can reduce stress [42,43,44].

Mindfulness, a form of meditation, is characterized as a way of paying attention in a purposeful way [42]. Intentionally directing one’s attention can be regarded as a self-regulatory practice and is often achieved through meditation [45]. While mindfulness meditation can refer to many practices, the core of these practices is the moment-to-moment awareness of one’s body and mind, without judgment or criticism. This is often done by focusing attention on one’s own breath, promoting cognitive control [46]. Focusing one’s attention on the present moment can reduce negative rumination and worry about the future, common in highly stressed individuals.

The attentional focus on breathing slowly and in a regulated fashion may help reduce stress as breathing has significant influence on self-regulatory processes such as cardiac modulation [47]. Individuals under high stress consistently exhibit erratic breathing patterns, while individuals under low stress are shown to exhibit slow, regulated breathing patterns [48, 49]. The relationship between the breath and cardiac modulation is bi-directional, in that changes in breath rate can also influence stress levels [50, 51]. Moreover, alterations in respiratory patterns are proven to affect an individual’s physiological and emotional arousal as well as perceptions of negative distress [50, 52, 53], while short-term modification of breathing patterns has been found to reduce the subjective experience of stress [54]. Solutions to the transfer problem, however, are more difficult to find or to practically realize. One would have to have therapeutic settings closely resemble or simulate the real-life situations that patients experience their problems in. This appears hard and costly to do but would, if realized, likely increase the efficacy of treatment. Indeed, the benefits of treatment are often the largest when such an approach is possible.

Various newly developed wearable biofeedback devices, however, may offer unexplored possibilities. Biofeedback involves observing one’s own physiological responses and modulating behavioral patterns in real time [55], possibly providing a solution to combat the transfer problem encountered by many health providers. Wearable biofeedback devices can deliver prompts based on continuously monitored physiological signals. Prompts, such as minor vibrations, text messages, or other cues, can provide real-time feedback cueing wearers to adjust unhealthy habits. These devices can remind wearers of their mental and physiological state and signal them to perform behavioral cues in real-life contexts at the exact moments they are actually experiencing certain emotions. Specifically, a signal to remind individuals to modulate their respiratory patterns may increase the transfer of learning. Wearable devices also have the advantage that they are becoming increasingly more affordable and available. Self- tracking of health factors such as weight, exercise, blood pressure, sleep, and blood sugar are now commonplace and can be provided at relatively low-cost [56].

Public-private disconnect

Furthermore, biofeedback interventions include modulation and direction of awareness towards one’s own physiology, akin to the process of mindfulness. Through feedback signals from the device, individuals are able to gain increased awareness of physiological processes and ideally gain control and modulate these processes. The use of biofeedback-aided interventions for stress and anxiety has proven successful with various populations including school aged children [43], college students [57], professionals [58], athletes [59], and individuals suffering from mental illnesses, (e.g., depression and post-traumatic stress disorder) [60,61,62]. A majority of evidence, however, utilizes non-portable biofeedback devices, which limits the ecological validity. Furthermore, non-portable biofeedback devices are costly and access to these devices is often restricted, requiring permission through medical offices or wellness centers. Hence, considering the limited literature on this topic, there is a clear need for more robust discussions on the feasibility of using these biofeedback devices within the context of randomized control trials (RCT).

In our current day and age, private companies are not required to provide detailed information regarding feasibility and effectiveness of released products, unless they are seeking FDA approval. While these devices are relatively inexpensive for individual users, the overall wearable market is growing and predicted to increase over the next few years, with global estimates ranging between $25 and $44 billion dollars, by 2020 [63,64,65]. The exponential growth of technology [66] has produced an ecosystem of relatively inexpensive technological products that may or may not provide real clinical value. Currently, there is a lack of incentivization for private companies to partner with researchers. Further, these health-related technological devices do not require clinical trials, to assess the efficacy, prior to making claims. Despite the large market for this industry, to the best of our knowledge, literature supporting the use of wearable biofeedback devices is often theoretical [67] or mechanical [68, 69] in nature, while applied research of products is scarce (despite a few studies, see [70, 71]). Moreover, we could not identify literature pairing wearable biofeedback devices with meditation as a treatment option. Coupling the lack of literature with the vast number of devices that exist on the market indicates a need to test the feasibility of these devices as well as evaluate the quality of participant adherence and collaboration with private companies.

The current study

The literature suggests a strong potential for meditation and biofeedback treatments in reducing stress [72, 73]. Further, because of mindfulness meditation’s focus on respiration and physiology, the integration of respiratory-based biofeedback and mindfulness meditation appears evident. Our study had two key objectives. Our primary objective related to feasibility and had two key components. First, we needed to assess adherence with the device. We will test this using the amount of missing data due to adherence. Second, we needed to assess the biofeedback company’s communication and transparency. This was important because if we showed the device was effective, we would need to determine the mechanism of action in a larger RCT. This objective could be tested by our research team gaining access to the algorithms and raw data. Our expectation was that both the participants and the biofeedback company with comply with our study design and data requirements in such as it would be feasible to run a larger scale study.

A secondary objective was to estimate the effectiveness of a wearable biofeedback device in addition to the effects of a mindfulness meditation intervention. This was important because if the device did not demonstrate any potential effectiveness, we may have to seek a different type of biofeedback device or question our theoretical assumptions. If there was evidence that the device conferred added benefit and that data feasibility was acceptable, we would design a larger and more elaborate study with a waitlist control group that would confirm these findings and also test more specifically for the mechanisms of improvement. We note that, due to the current experimental design of the pilot, we cannot make claims regarding the effectiveness of the mindfulness treatment nor the mechanism of action. We can only make claims as to the estimated added effectiveness of the specific biofeedback device tested.

Methods

Participants

University students who self-identified as experiencing chronic stress were recruited through flyers and a campus wide email on a large Southwestern United States campus between February and July of 2017. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) at least at least 18 years of age and be a student at time of testing, (2) self-identify as highly stressed. Exclusion criteria included (1) major psychiatric or developmental disorders besides depression or anxiety, (2) limited proficiency in English language.

Sample size

Figure 1 shows a detailed chart in accordance with the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) reporting standards of the flow of participants through our study. Eligible participants were randomized, before they visited the lab using a simple randomization scheme through the website www.randomization.com, into two arms: a meditation treatment with biofeedback group (the biofeedback experimental group; final n = 21) and a meditation without biofeedback group (no-biofeedback control group; final n = 18). As no previous studies have been conducted on this topic, no effect size could be identified for power analysis. Additionally, as this was a pilot study, we decided to keep the groups to approximately 20 each.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram of participants

Lab procedure

Eligibility screening was conducted via a brief phone call. Following the eligibility screening, participants visited the psychology lab, located on the campus of the university, for their first visit where they signed informed consent before data collection began. First, participants completed a set of questionnaires relating to demographic data and mental-health functioning (see details in the “Measures” section). Next, participants completed cognitive and behavioral tasks, which are not examined in the current study and are to be reported elsewhere. Following these tasks, a relaxation training session was conducted which utilized mindfulness-based relaxation techniques [42, 46]. Randomization was conducted by the project manager during the mindfulness training session to determine group placement of participant into one of two groups: (1) control group without a biofeedback device; (2) experimental group with biofeedback device. The first lab visit lasted approximately 65 min. Participants were also notified that a nightly survey would be sent at approximately 8 pm each night of the intervention. After 2 weeks, participants returned to the lab for their second visit, in which outcome data was collected via questionnaires.

If the participant was randomized into the experimental group, the research assistant would then conduct hands-on instruction with the participant on the use of the biofeedback device. This included information on how to properly pair the device to their phone, how to charge the device at night, and a high-level overview of the app’s functionality. After the participant’s device was properly paired and basic functionality of the device was understood and verified, the research assistant then provided the participant with the gift card and provided them information about daily surveys to be delivered each night. If the participant was placed in the control group, the research assistant would provide the participant with a gift card and ask them to fill out a daily survey that will be texted to them each night for the duration of the treatment. Participants returned to the laboratory after a 2-week interval. Participants received a monetary compensation of $40 ($20 pre-training and $20 post-training follow-up) for their time in the form of an Amazon gift-card. The same lab procedures were followed for the second lab visit. The local ethics review board approved the study. (Protocol reference: #IRB2015-0786D). This study was preregistered at ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT02837016. Registered 19 July 2016, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02837016

Mindfulness meditation training

Both arms received mindfulness meditation training. The mindfulness relaxation training script was designed by a clinical psychology graduate student and executed by trained undergraduate research assistants. In this training, the research assistant began by providing a brief overview of the theory behind mindfulness meditation and reviewed why it can work in controlling stress. First, the research assistants discussed how to breathe slowly and deeply for relaxation [47, 50] and monitored the participants breathing habits to ensure proper breathing techniques were established before moving on. Then, the research assistants discussed current theory and research on mindfulness meditation. After that, the research assistant left the room, while a 20-min mindfulness meditation audio was played on the computer. The audio emphasized awareness of the body and regulation of the breath [42, 46, 50]. Lastly the research assistant returned to the room and asked the participant if s/he has any follow-up questions. Additionally, the research assistant discussed how the participant might be able to integrate these techniques in their daily lives.

Wearable respiratory-based biofeedback device

A biofeedback device was used that collects continuous respiration data and claims to provide real-time feedback through a messenger app when breathing patterns indicate tension. The device, which can sit comfortably on someone’s hip, will buzz and vibrate and send a message to the wearer’s phone when it detects erratic, defined by fast and highly variable, breathing patterns. The message may, for example, gently remind participants to mind their breath and do a deep breathing exercise to help alleviate stress. This device was chosen based on affordability, unobtrusiveness, and its focus on respiration.

Prior to data collection, the authors discussed the design and goal of the study with a senior member of the company. We received verbal confirmation we would be able to receive back end data, such as notifications per day, activity on the app, types of alerts received. This data could be anonymized based on device ID to allow for data matching. Further, we were notified that we would be able to also receive information about the specific algorithm used. After data was collected, initial findings were presented and shared with the senior member of the company.

Measures

A scale was developed in order to gather information on possible covariates. The scale was administered after physiological hookup. Frequency of exercise was assessed using item ‘I exercise often,’ measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Whether participants were in treatment or taking medication were assessed using items, ‘Are you currently receiving any treatment for your stress?’ and ‘Are you prescribed any medications?” both measured through dichotomous, yes or no, responses. Alcohol usage was measured using item ‘How many days a week do you typically drink alcohol?’, with a scale between 1 and 7. Last, using the Macarthur Scale of subjective Social Status [74], individuals were asked to place themselves on a ladder based on their perceived social standing relative to their community on a scale between 1 and 10. Additionally, at the end of the lab visit, both groups were asked to answer questions on their motivation and expectation to change using items ‘How much do you expect a positive change?,’ and ‘How motivated are you to try new things to change?” on a scale from 1—very little to 10—very much.

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)

The STAI is a self-report questionnaire that measures trait and state anxiety in adults [75, 76]. The term anxiety is used to describe both an unpleasant state (state-anxiety) as well as an individual’s relatively stable personality trait that describes an individual’s proneness towards anxiety (trait-anxiety) [76]. Please note that no clinical or diagnostic procedures were conducted which means findings may not generalize to individuals with anxiety disorders. Our findings can only speak to the symptomology associated with trait and state anxiety. For these reasons, we will adopt the more generic term ‘stress’ to describe the symptoms in this manuscript. The STAI is considered a widely used, reliable, and valid measure of stress and its symptomology [75,76,77].

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS)

The DERS is a self-report questionnaire that measures multiple aspects of and difficulties with emotion regulation [78]. The DERS provides a total score as an indicator of emotion regulation difficulty, as well as subscales specific to emotion regulation strategies, non-acceptance of negative emotional responses, emotional clarity, impulse control, goal-directed behavior, and emotional awareness. This measure is a widely used and generally considered a reliable, and well-validated measure capturing difficulties in emotion regulation [78, 79].

Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)

The PSS is a self-report measure of how stressful one perceives his or her own life [80]. This scale was used as evidence indicates the importance and role of cognitive appraisal in perceptions of stress [81]. The PSS provides a single total score to reflect this stress level. This measure is a well-published, and generally considered a reliable and valid measure of perceived stress [80,81,82]. During conversion from paper to digital format, an error occurred causing item seven (‘In the last month, how often have you been able to control irritations in your life?’) to be left off from the questionnaire. Thus, this item is not reported or reflected within PSS pre-training or post-training scores.

Compliance

A nightly survey, for the entire 2-week intervention, was sent to both groups at 8 pm via text message through a service called Survey Signal [83]. This measure was used as an approximation for device-adherence. We asked participants to answer this survey as soon as they received it. The survey asked questions about whether a participant had meditated that day. A participant’s response, of either yes or no, was used as a measure of compliance (see Table 3). All continuously enrolled participants were sent both minimum and maximum number of fourteen surveys, one for each day they were enrolled in the study. Additional adherence was to be estimated based on the biofeedback company’s user data; however, the one-time data sent by the company resulted in a low match rate with our device identifiers and the company then became unresponsive despite multiple attempts to follow up.

Analysis

SPSS version 24.0 [84] was used for the analyses. Repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was used to compare the different treatment arms across time for each of the main outcome measures. For the main analyses, the STAI will be used to index changes in anxiety, the PSS will index changes in stress levels, whereas the DERS will be used to measure changes in the ability of emotion regulation. To assess group differences in age, sex, ethnicity, and grade type, chi-square and independent sample t test were used. Analyses of variances (ANOVAs) were used to test group differences in income, state-anxiety, and trait-anxiety. 95% confidence intervals (CI) are provided for the change-scores across session for each group (control and experimental) and group for each session time point (pre- and post-treatment).

No missing data existed on major variables (less than 5% for all measures) and no outliers were identified (scores above 2 standard deviations from the mean). Each individual was tested and included only once within our sample and analysis. Additionally, we have no reason to suspect that measurements for one subject may have influenced or is related to the measurements of other subjects and participants were explicitly instructed not to discuss the research study with anyone else.

Results

The campus can be characterized as diverse, featuring students from urban and rural areas and different ethnic and socioeconomic backgrounds. Table 1 shows demographic data for the final sample broken down by group. The baseline values for age, gender, ethnicity, grade level, income, and state-anxiety and trait-anxiety levels did not differ among the groups.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics by group

Additionally, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [73] used to confirm high stress and anxiety levels in our sample through comparison with college-age normative scores. State-Anxiety refers to an unpleasant state, while trait-anxiety describes an individual’s proneness towards symptoms of anxiety [73]. Male and female participants scored in the 81st and 79th percentiles for state-anxiety, respectively, while both groups scored above the 90th percentile for trait-anxiety symptoms. Scores at or above the 75th percentiles, for both State and Trait-Anxiety, indicate clinically significant levels of anxiety that may affect their ability to function [73]. Again, chi-square and ANOVA results indicated no group differences among our sample (Table 1).

Preliminary analysis examined group differences on sociodemographic measures between group. See Table 2 for means and standard deviations for pre-training time and post-training time for both groups. No group differences on major descriptive variables were found. Eight additional items were examined: frequency of exercise, in treatment, taking medication, alcohol usage, social ladder perception, motivation to change, expectation to change, and compliance. Chi-square goodness of fit and t tests were used to assess group differences for each of the above variables. No significant group differences were found on all above variables (see Table 3) indicating no statistically significant group differences between the control versus experimental group on possible covariates including compliance.

Table 2 Means, standard deviations, and 95% paired means confidence intervals for dependent variables by group (control, experimental) and time (pre- and post-training)
Table 3 Comparison of additional training related items between groups

Our first objective regarding feasibility was to test compliance. While compliance was not statistically different between groups, the low average rate of compliance in both the control (M = 9.3, SD = 4.6) and experimental (M = 8.1, SD = 5.6) groups indicate our daily device-adherence measure to be insufficient without supplementary data on actual utilization. As for our secondary objective, the company failed to provide adequate supplementary data needed. We did not receive access to the raw data or gain  insight into the algorithms used to compute “stress level” from the breathing signal.

Figure 2 shows the data pre-training and post-training for our four outcome measures: the state anxiety, trait anxiety, perceived stress, and difficulty in emotion regulation.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scores for dependent variables state-anxiety, trait-anxiety, perceived stress, and difficulties in emotion regulation shown per group (experimental and control) and at both time points (pre- and post-training)

To measure the effects of the wearable biofeedback device on state anxiety, trait anxiety, perceived stress, and emotion regulation, four separate 2 (group: experimental, control) × 2 (time: pre, post) RM-ANOVA with the outcomes measures as the dependent variable were conducted (Table 4). These analyses showed strong main effects of time for state anxiety (F(1,37) = 9.370, p = .004, ηp2 = .202, trait anxiety (F(1,37) = 23.602, p = .000 , ηp2 = .389, perceived stress (F(1,37) = 25.65, p = .000 , ηp2 = .409, and emotion regulation (F(1,37) = 5.343, p = .026 , ηp2 = .126. No main effects of group were identified for state anxiety (F(1,37) = 3.089, p = .087, trait anxiety (F(1,37) = .320, p = .575, perceived stress (F(1,37) = .65, p =.424, or emotion regulation (F(1,37) = .016, p = .901. No group × time interaction effect was statistically significant (all p’s > .237). Table 4 shows the relevant statistics and effects sizes from the RM-ANOVA.

Table 4 RM-ANOVA results for dependent variables

Discussion

Data feasibility

Though participant compliance was not different between groups, the low average rate of compliance pointed towards a need for the biofeedback company’s actual utilization data to supplement our daily survey. However, due to lack of shared data regarding user behavior in relation to these devices and low transparency, we determine that the feasibility of such a study without full collaboration with private companies is low. The feasibility of future studies requires full collaboration with companies that includes detailed, agreed-upon procedures for data sharing. It is essential to ensure data from the back-end, such as verification that participants were in fact wearing the device, number of notifications provided by the company, as well as the user’s activity once a notification was received is provided in order to understand how this behavior interacts with other measures taken at the lab. Without this data, adherence can only be estimated and assumed. While we hoped that a randomized design would control for consumer behavior surrounding these devices, feasibility of the study was ultimately jeopardized. For future studies, we recommend formal written partnerships, as well as a detailed data passback policy to ensure transparency and successful data matching by participant. Additionally, future studies could design to accommodate for the low rate of participant adherence. For example, text messages could be sent as reminders on an intermittent basis. Furthermore, biofeedback devices that do not require frequent charging and that are waterproof may work best for younger samples so as to decrease actions needed for better adherence.

Potential device effectiveness

Our secondary objective, examining whether this particular wearable respiratory-based biofeedback device would reduce stress more effectively than mindfulness meditation alone, ultimately showed no difference. Results of our small-scale trial showed that participants in the experimental biofeedback group were not different from the control group in their change of levels of stress before and after the treatment period. Our study did find that both the experimental and control group reported reduced levels of stress across the treatment period. As we did not utilize a proper control group to test for the effect of meditation training alone (e.g., a waitlist or active control group), it is possible that the beneficial effect could be due to the administration of the meditation training which was provided for both arms. These findings suggest that the wearable respiratory-biofeedback device did not have an added benefit in reducing stress above and beyond mindfulness meditation training and that the use of this specific device does not prove feasible for a larger scale study.

The current findings can be explained in several ways. First, it is possible that the device-algorithm was not working optimally. From a measurement perspective, accurately capturing respiratory information in real-life situations, in which participants are constantly moving and speaking, is challenging [85, 86]. Being able to link those signals back to specific emotions, among a possible myriad of other movements and states, may simply have been too complex. Although some basis for the wearable device was provided by the company [87, 88], these papers did not constitute an actual validation of the algorithms of the device. Unfortunately, we were unable to obtain the algorithms and, as such, the scientific contribution of our study is more utilitarian than mechanistic. Second, it is possible that the device was accurately capturing the respiratory signal but that the breath is simply not a good way to measure stress physiology in real-life situations. Currently, respiratory measurements are noted to be widely subjective depending on methodology, and previous studies have exposed reliability and validity issues relating to respiratory rate measurements [89]. Furthermore, to properly measure the breath, information on the rate, the volume, and the pattern must be reviewed in tandem. And while RSA and HRV are well validated in the literature, the introduction of new ways of measuring, such as apps or electrical equipment within cell phones, combined with possible artifacts generated through these new data captures, requires much more evidence to support the use of these devices for a clinical population. Third, these findings could indicate that the mindfulness meditation intervention paired with the breath regulation instruction alone may provide effective treatment, without the need for the use of a wearable biofeedback device.

Our results are difficult to situate in the current biofeedback literature. To the best of our knowledge, Gutierrez-Osuna’s group at Texas A&M University is only other group that conducted research utilizing a wearable biofeedback device capturing breathing rate as a means to influence stress. They showed that breath-based biofeedback training was more effective in inducing relaxation in participants than electrodermal- and cardiovascular-based biofeedback [90]. Though our studies are similar in some respects, they differ in that their study used the device as a training tool to remediate stress through a video-game interface, whereas in our study the device was used for the recognition of stress physiological markers.

Limitations

Further limitations of the study should be considered in the development of a full-scaled study. One limitation is the lack of a waitlist control group (or active control group for the meditation alone) precluding statements regarding the effectiveness of the meditation. No waitlist control was utilized as we were primarily interested in the additive effects of the biofeedback device as opposed to the effects of meditation intervention per se. This study was planned as a phase 1 exploratory study testing the feasibility of a particular wearable biofeedback device that we intended to use in future, more detailed, studies to assess the general overall of the treatment compared to a waitlist or to determine the mechanism of action if results were positive.

Despite this limitation, we do point out that other studies which have utilized active control by meditation designs have found meditation interventions to generate positive effect sizes ranging from 0.30 to 0.67 [91, 92]. Also, previous studies conducted with students have found outcome variables relating to stress to either remain stable or increase in intensity between pre-test and post-test [85, 89]. As students in our study showed statistically significant decreases in stress measures, we believe, albeit speculatively, this could suggest that the meditation treatment was effective for high stress college students.

Furthermore, a few statistical limitations must be mentioned. We decided to run this study as a pilot to a future larger study as no previous studies have been conducted on our specific variables of interest and no effect size could be estimated. Because of this, the recruitment of highly stressed individuals combined with the smaller sample size may have affected the variability of our outcome variables reducing statistical power to detect meaningful findings. Additionally, it is also possible that regression to the mean, as well as test-retest effects could account for changes in both the test and control groups.

We also note that the present sample consisted of a non-clinical sample of college students who self-reported experiencing high levels of stress. It is possible findings could be different if a clinical sample was targeted in which people had confirmed diagnoses. Future clinical trials with diagnosed patients suffering from stress could help confirm the present findings.

Finally, we cannot exclude the possibility that the device was working properly but that the participants were simply not adhering to, or misunderstood, the instructions. We do believe this is unlikely as participants received more information on the use of the device than if they bought the device from the store. Furthermore, our team also made sure to demonstrate how to properly wear and use the device. Given the aforementioned limitations, it is important that future studies rigorously evaluate the validity of wearable biofeedback devices against currently existing, well-validated treatment options.

Implications

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first of its kind in studying a wearable respiratory-based biofeedback device in conjunction with mindfulness meditation through an RCT design. Strengths of this study include the application of these treatment options within an ecologically valid setting and the rigorous design using an active control group. Our findings suggest the biofeedback device provided no additive benefit to mindfulness training for stressed individuals which could be concerning as individuals with high stress levels seeking clinical solutions may be more susceptible to marketing messages. Furthermore, the authors caution against the use of commercially produced wearable biofeedback devices without Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approval for use in clinical settings and for patient monitoring. However, ultimately, as various limitations must be considered, future testing is recommended. Finally, we decided not to continue with the present company due to challenges related to transparency and information sharing.

Conclusions

The goal of this study was to test the feasibility of using the biofeedback device, a respiration-based wearable biofeedback device claiming to reduce stress in a sample of college students. As such, our study is among the first to independently test commercially available biometric wearables. Our findings showed no added effectiveness of the specific respiratory-based biofeedback device we tested in reducing stress. Thus, we believe that these companies should comply with the same FDA standards required for medical companies. As it stands now, the lack of regulation can lead to public confusion and false hope in health-related industries. Furthermore, the validation of a commercial product proved difficult due to limited access to participants' behavior and adherence data regarding the device from the device maker. We assess that the feasibility of such a study proves difficult without closer collaborations between independent scientists and corporation in the research and development of such devices. With this, we would like to advocate for the collaboration of the FDA, academics, and private companies in the development of evidence-based regulations that will allow for a successful certification process that can bolster technological innovation while maintaining product quality, safety, and effectiveness. Ultimately, academia’s role as research and learning institutions can be strengthened through the government’s data collection and regulatory process capabilities, while industry can help close the gap between research and application through the development of products. Moreover, the open science research paradigm [93] can promote data sharing, preregistration, and funding source databases, and foster trust and collaboration across industries and disciplines.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework repository, https://osf.io/hec7a/

Abbreviations

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

CBT:

Cognitive behavioral therapy

DERS:

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale

FDA:

Food and Drug Administration

HRV:

Heart rate variability

PSS:

Perceived Stress Scale

RCT:

Randomized control trial

RM–ANOVA:

Repeated measures analysis of variance

RSA:

Respiratory sinus arrhythmia

STAI:

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

References

  1. American College Health Association. American College Health Association-National College Health Assessment II: Reference group executive summary fall 2017. 2018

    Google Scholar 

  2. Andrews B, Wilding JM. The relation of depression and anxiety to life-stress and achievement in students. British Journal of Psychology. 2004 Nov 1;95(4):509–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bayram N, Bilgel N. The prevalence and socio-demographic correlations of depression, anxiety and stress among a group of university students. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology. 2008 Aug 1;43(8):667–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Beiter R, Nash R, McCrady M, Rhoades D, Linscomb M, Clarahan M, Sammut S. The prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and stress in a sample of college students. Journal of affective disorders. 2015 Mar 1;173:90–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Boynton Health Service. Health and health-related behaviors, University of Minnesota-Twin Cities students.[Internet]. Regents of the University of Minnesota [cited 2018Oct1]. Available from: https://boynton.umn.edu/sites/boynton.umn.edu/files/2017-09/2010CSHSReport_MinnesotaPostsecondaryStudenVeterans.pdf

  6. Mahmoud JS, Staten RT, Hall LA, Lennie TA. The relationship among young adult college students’ depression, anxiety, stress, demographics, life satisfaction, and coping styles. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2012;33(3):149–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gallagher RP. National survey of college counseling centers 2014. The International Association of Counseling Services, Inc; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Seipp B. Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety Research. 1991;4(1):27–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Shields N. Stress, active coping, and academic performance among persisting and nonpersisting college students. J Applied Biobehav Res. 2001;6(2):65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Thomas CL, Cassady JC, Heller ML. The influence of emotional intelligence, cognitive test anxiety, and coping strategies on undergraduate academic performance. Learning and Individual Differences. 2017;55:40–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Lee J, Jang S. An exploration of stress and satisfaction in college students. Services Marketing Quarterly. 2015;36(3):245–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Stader SR, Hokanson JE. Psychosocial antecedents of depressive symptoms: An evaluation using daily experiences methodology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107(1):17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Härter MC, Conway KP, Merikangas KR. Associations between anxiety disorders and physical illness. European archives of psychiatry and clinical neuroscience. 2003;253(6):313–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Rawson HE, Bloomer K, Kendall A. Stress, anxiety, depression, and physical illness in college students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 1994;155(3):321–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Blanco C, Okuda M, Wright C, Hasin DS, Grant BF, Liu SM, Olfson M. Mental health of college students and their non–college-attending peers: results from the national epidemiologic study on alcohol and related conditions. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008;65(12):1429–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Cranford JA, Eisenberg D, Serras AM. Substance use behaviors, mental health problems, and use of mental health services in a probability sample of college students. Addictive behaviors. 2009;34(2):134–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Flynn HA. Comparison of cross-sectional and daily reports in studying the relationship between depression and use of alcohol in response to stress in college students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24(1):48–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Tavolacci MP, Ladner J, Grigioni S, Richard L, Villet H, Dechelotte P. Prevalence and association of perceived stress, substance use and behavioral addictions: a cross-sectional study among university students in France, 2009–2011. BMC public health. 2013;13(1):724.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Valerio TD, Kim MJ, Sexton-Radek K. Association of stress, general health, and alcohol use with poor sleep quality among US college students. Am J Health Education. 2016;47(1):17–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Wichianson JR, Bughi SA, Unger JB, Spruijt-Metz D, Nguyen-Rodriguez ST. Perceived stress, coping and night-eating in college students. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress. 2009;25(3):235–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Patrick BC, Skinner EA, Connell JP. What motivates children’s behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the academic domain. Journal of Personality and social Psychology. 1993;65(4):781.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Salehi A, Harris N, Coyne E, Sebar B. Perceived control and self-efficacy, subjective well-being and lifestyle behaviours in young Iranian women. J Health Psychology. 2016;21(7):1415–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Bandelow B, Lichte T, Rudolf S, Wiltink J, Beutel EM. The diagnosis of and treatment recommendations for anxiety disorders. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International. 2014;111(27-28):473.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Bandelow B, Michaelis S, Wedekind D. Treatment of anxiety disorders. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience. 2017;19(2):93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Cuijpers P, Cristea IA, Karyotaki E, Reijnders M, Huibers MJ. How effective are cognitive behavior therapies for major depression and anxiety disorders? A meta-analytic update of the evidence. World Psychiatry. 2016;15(3):245–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Cuijpers P, Sijbrandij M, Koole S, Huibers M, Berking M, Andersson G. Psychological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical psychology review. 2014;34(2):130–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cuijpers P, Sijbrandij M, Koole SL, Andersson G, Beekman AT, Reynolds CF III. Adding psychotherapy to antidepressant medication in depression and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(1):56–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Norton PJ, Price EC. A meta-analytic review of adult cognitive-behavioral treatment outcome across the anxiety disorders. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 2007;195(6):521–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Eisenberg D, Golberstein E, Gollust SE. Help-seeking and access to mental health care in a university student population. Medical care. 2007;1:594–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Eisenberg D, Downs MF, Golberstein E, Zivin K. Stigma and help seeking for mental health among college students. Medical Care Research and Review. 2009;66(5):522–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hunt J, Eisenberg D. Mental health problems and help-seeking behavior among college students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2010 Jan 31;46(1):3–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Mawjee K, Woltering S, Tannock R. Working memory training in post-secondary students with ADHD: a randomized controlled study. PloS one. 2015;10(9):e0137173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Woltering S, Lishak V, Hodgson N, Granic I, Zelazo PD. Executive function in children with externalizing and comorbid internalizing behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2016;57(1):30–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005;1:167–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Woltering S, Shi Q. On the neuroscience of self-regulation in children with disruptive behavior problems: implications for education. Rev Educational Res. 2016;86(4):1085–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Dziegielewski SF, Turnage B, Roest-Marti S. Addressing stress with social work students: A controlled evaluation. Journal of Social Work Education. 2004;40(1):105–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Broderick PC. Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2005;29(5):501–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Ortner CN, Kilner SJ, Zelazo PD. Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motivation Emotion. 2007;31(4):271–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Jha AP, Krompinger J, Baime MJ. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7(2):109–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Erisman SM, Roemer L. A preliminary investigation of the effects of experimentally induced mindfulness on emotional responding to film clips. Emotion. 2010;10(1):72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Urry HL, Van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, Kalin NH, Thurow ME, Schaefer HS, Jackson CA, Frye CJ, Greischar LL, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ. Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. J Neuroscience. 2006;26(16):4415–25.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Baer RA. Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychol. 2003;10(2):125–43.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Semple RJ, Droutman V, Reid BA. Mindfulness goes to school: Things learned (so far) from research and real-world experiences. Psychology in the Schools. 2017;54(1):29–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Van Dam, Nicholas T., Marieke K. Van Vugt, David R. Vago, Laura Schmalzl, Clifford D. Saron, Andrew Olendzki, Ted Meissner et al. "Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation." Perspectives on psychological science 13, no. 1 (2018): 36-61.

  45. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Segal ZV, Abbey S, Speca M, Velting D, Devins G. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical psychology: Science and practice. 2004 Sep;11(3):230–41.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Desrosiers A, Vine V, Klemanski DH, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: common and distinct mechanisms of action. Depression and anxiety. 2013;30(7):654–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Hirsch JA, Bishop B. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia in humans: how breathing pattern modulates heart rate. Am J Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology. 1981;241(4):H620–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Porges SW, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Maiti AK. Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the society for research in child development. 1994;59(2-3):167–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of affective disorders. 2000 Dec 2;61(3):201–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Homma I, Masaoka Y. Breathing rhythms and emotions. Experimental physiology. 2008 Sep 1;93(9):1011–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Boiten FA, Frijda NH, Wientjes CJ. Emotions and respiratory patterns: review and critical analysis. Int J Psychophysiology. 1994;17(2):103–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. McCaul KD, Solomon S, Holmes DS. Effects of paced respiration and expectations on physiological and psychological responses to threat. J Personality Soc Psychol. 1979;37(4):564.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Masaoka Y, Homma I. Anxiety and respiratory patterns: their relationship during mental stress and physical load. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997;27(2):153–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Grossman P. Respiration, stress, and cardiovascular function. Psychophysiology. 1983;20(3):284–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. McKee M. Biofeedback: An overview in the context of heart-brain medicine. Cleveland Clinic J Med. 2008;75:S31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Swan M. The quantified self: Fundamental disruption in big data science and biological discovery. Big Data. 2013;1(2):85–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Heaman D. The quieting response (QR): a modality for reduction of psychophysiologic stress in nursing students. J Nursing Education. 1995;34(1):5–9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Horgan K, Howard S, Gardiner-Hyland F. Pre-service Teachers and Stress During Microteaching: An Experimental Investigation of the Effectiveness of Relaxation Training with Biofeedback on Psychological and Physiological Indices of Stress. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback. 2018;43(3):217–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Dupee M, Werthner P. Managing the stress response: the use of biofeedback and neurofeedback with Olympic athletes. Biofeedback. 2011;39(3):92–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Karavidas MK, Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Marin H, Buyske S, Malinovsky I, Radvanski D, Hassett A. Preliminary results of an open label study of heart rate variability biofeedback for the treatment of major depression. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback. 2007;32(1):19–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Zucker TL, Samuelson KW, Muench F, Greenberg MA, Gevirtz RN. The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: a pilot study. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback. 2009;34(2):135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Reiner R. Integrating a portable biofeedback device into clinical practice for patients with anxiety disorders: Results of a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 2008;33(1):55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Wilson S. Wearables Market to Be Worth $25 Billion by 2019 [Internet]. CCS Insight. [cited 2018Oct1]. Available from: https://www.ccsinsight.com/press/company-news/2332-wearables-market-to-be-worth-25-billion-by-2019-reveals-ccs-insight

  64. Tractica. Wearable device revenue worldwide 2016-2022 | Statistic [Internet]. Statista. [cited 2018Oct1]. Available from: https://www.statista.com/statistics/610447/wearable-device-revenue-worldwide/

  65. Zheng J, Shen Y, Zhang Z, Wu T, Zhang G, Lu H. Emerging wearable medical devices towards personalized healthcare. InProceedings of the 8th International Conference on Body Area Networks 2013 Sep 30 (pp. 427-431). ICST (Institute for Computer Sciences, Social-Informatics and Telecommunications Engineering).

  66. Moore GE. Cramming more components onto integrated circuits, Reprinted from Electronics, volume 38, number 8, April 19, 1965, pp. 114 ff. IEEE Solid-State Circuits Society Newsletter. 2006 Sep;11(3):33-5.

  67. Munro BJ, Campbell TE, Wallace GG, Steele JR. The intelligent knee sleeve: A wearable biofeedback device. Sensors and Actuators B: Chemical. 2008;131(2):541–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Jo E, Lewis K, Directo D, Kim MJ, Dolezal BA. Validation of biofeedback wearables for photoplethysmographic heart rate tracking. Journal of sports science & medicine. 2016;15(3):540.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Costa J, Adams AT, Jung MF, Guimbretière F, Choudhury T. EmotionCheck: leveraging bodily signals and false feedback to regulate our emotions. In: Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing: ACM; 2016. p. 758–69.

  70. MacLean D, Roseway A, Czerwinski M. MoodWings: a wearable biofeedback device for real-time stress intervention. InProceedings of the 6th international conference on PErvasive Technologies Related to Assistive Environments 2013 May 29 (p. 66). ACM.

  71. van der Zwan JE, de Vente W, Huizink AC, Bögels SM, de Bruin EI. Physical activity, mindfulness meditation, or heart rate variability biofeedback for stress reduction: a randomized controlled trial. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback. 2015;40(4):257–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Ratanasiripong P, Park JF, Ratanasiripong N, Kathalae D. Stress and anxiety management in nursing students: biofeedback and mindfulness meditation. Journal of Nursing Education. 2015;54(9):520–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL. State-trait anxiety inventory for adults: Manual and sample: Manual, instrument and scoring guide: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.

  74. Operario D, Adler NE, Williams DR. Subjective social status: Reliability and predictive utility for global health. Psychology Health. 2004;19(2):237–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Barnes LL, Harp D, Jung WS. Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Educational Psychological Measurement. 2002;62(4):603–18.

  76. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1983. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. 1983.

  77. Jones KI, Amawi F, Bhalla A, Peacock O, Williams JP, Lund JN. Assessing surgeon stress when operating using heart rate variability and the State Trait Anxiety Inventory: will surgery be the death of us? Colorectal Disease. 2015;17(4):335–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment. 2004;26(1):41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Ritschel LA, Tone EB, Schoemann AM, Lim NE. Psychometric properties of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale across demographic groups. Psychological Assessment. 2015;27(3):944.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;1:385–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Lee EH. Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian nursing research. 2012;6(4):121–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Roberti JW, Harrington LN, Storch EA. Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the perceived stress scale. Journal of College Counseling. 2006;9(2):135–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. SurveySignal, LLC. Chicago, IL. 2015.

  84. Corp IB. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 24.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. 2016.

  85. Porges SW, Byrne EA. Research methods for measurement of heart rate and respiration. Biological psychology. 1992;34(2-3):93–130.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Kressbach M. Breath work: mediating health through breathing apps and wearable technologies. New Review of Film and Television Studies. 2018;16(2):184–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Leape C, Fong A, Ratwani RM. Heuristic usability evaluation of wearable mental state monitoring sensors for healthcare environments. InProceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting 2016 Sep (Vol. 60, No. 1, pp. 583-587). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

  88. Lovett PB, Buchwald JM, Stürmann K, Bijur P. The vexatious vital: neither clinical measurements by nurses nor an electronic monitor provides accurate measurements of respiratory rate in triage. Annals of emergency medicine. 2005;45(1):68–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Folke M, Cernerud L, Ekström M, Hök B. Critical review of non-invasive respiratory monitoring in medical care. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing. 2003;41(4):377–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Parnandi A, Gutierrez-Osuna R. Physiological modalities for relaxation skill transfer in biofeedback games. IEEE journal of biomedical and health informatics. 2017;21(2):361–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of psychosomatic research. 2004;57(1):35–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Rosenzweig S, Reibel DK, Greeson JM, Brainard GC, Hojat M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction lowers psychological distress in medical students. Teaching and learning in medicine. 2003;15(2):88–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Friesike S, Widenmayer B, Gassmann O, Schildhauer T. Opening science: towards an agenda of open science in academia and industry. The Journal of Technology Transfer. 2015;40(4):581–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Neyra Hernandez and Johanna Song for design feedback as well as data collection on this project.

Funding

This project was internally funded by professional development funds (Woltering) and student research awards by the College of Education and Human Development (Lin).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SW is senior author of this manuscript. BL and SW conceived of the study and made significant contributions to the overall study design and specific research questions. All authors had input on specifics of the design of the intervention, the content of the intervention materials, and the training of the implementers. BL drafted the manuscript. SW and CP provided critical revisions and contributed isolated sections. All authors read and approved of the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven Woltering.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Our protocol has been approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Texas A&M University (Protocol reference: #IRB2015-0786D).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

CONSORT extension for Pilot and Feasibility Trials Checklist-Stress RCT Pilot.doc CONSORT Extension Checklist

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lin, B., Prickett, C. & Woltering, S. Feasibility of using a biofeedback device in mindfulness training - a pilot randomized controlled trial. Pilot Feasibility Stud 7, 84 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-021-00807-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-021-00807-1

Keywords