- Study Protocol
- Open access
- Published:
The relationship between spatiotemporal gait parameters and cognitive function in healthy adults: protocol for a cross-sectional study
Pilot and Feasibility Studies volume 8, Article number: 154 (2022)
Abstract
Background
Motor dysfunctions, such as slower walking speed, precede the occurrence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment, suggesting that walking parameters are effective biomarkers for detecting early sub-clinical cognitive risk. It is often also concurrent with self-complained cognitive dysfunction, called motoric cognitive risk (MCR) syndrome. Our preliminary study found several walking parameters, obtained by a three-dimensional motion capture system, to be correlated with computer-based assessments of various cognitive function modalities, although the sample size was small. The Cognitive-Gait (CoGait) Database Project, described in the current protocol, aims to establish a database of multi-dimensional walking and cognitive performance data, collected from a large sample of healthy participants, crucial for detecting early sub-clinical cognitive risk.
Methods
We will recruit healthy volunteers, 20 years or older, without any neurological musculoskeletal or psychiatric disorders. The estimated sample size is 450 participants, including a 10% attrition rate. Using computer-based cognitive assessments, participants will perform six tasks: (i) the simple reaction time task, (ii) Go/No-Go task, (iii) Stroop Color–Word Test, (iv) N-back test, (v) Trail Making Test, and (vi) digit span test. We will also conduct paper-based cognitive assessments such as the Mini-Mental State Examination, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, and the Geriatric Depression Scale-15 for assessing MCR. Gait will be measured through joint kinematics and global positioning in participants’ lower legs while walking at a comfortable and faster pace, using pants with an inertial measurement unit-based three-dimensional motion capture system. Finally, we will establish a prediction model for various cognitive performance modalities based on walking performance.
Discussion
This will be the first study to reveal the relationship between walking and cognitive performance using multi-dimensional data collected from a large sample of healthy adults, from the general population. Despite certain methodological limitations such as the accuracy of measurements, the CoGait database is expected to be the standard value for both walking and cognitive functions, supporting the evaluation of psychomotor function in early sub-clinical cognitive risk identification, including motoric-cognitive risk syndrome.
Background
Motoric-cognitive risk (MCR) syndrome, characterized by self-reported cognitive complaints and slower walking speed, is associated with an increased risk for dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [1,2,3,4]. Detection of MCR syndrome can facilitate early intervention, such as pharmacological treatment, rehabilitation for sustained cognitive and physical functions, and prevention of MCI and dementia [5,6,7,8,9]. In addition to patient education for lifestyle modification, addressing issues such as diet, physical, and social activities reduces the risk of these cognitive disorders [10,11,12,13,14,15]. In a recent survey, the incidence rate of MCR in people in their 60s was 54.9 per 1000 persons, which strongly suggests that preventive activities for these cognitive disorders should be started at working age, before the initial presentation of early cognitive decline [16].
There are many risk factors for dementia, such as genetic factors, lifestyle habits, sleep quality, education, and physical and social activities, even in the absence of detectable cognitive risk [6, 7, 10, 11, 17,18,19]. Thus, we argue that cognitive risk screening in healthy participants requires additional multimodal parameters. Establishing a database of multimodal parameters that include these risk factors is necessary to distinguish individuals with higher cognitive risks from the general population.
Specifically, walking performance is the most notable marker of cognitive risk [20, 21]. Although walking speed is a known risk factor for cognitive decline, what domains of walking parameters (i.e., cadence, speed, toe clearance, joint angle) relate to early cognitive decline remain unknown. Therefore, a motion capture technique that can collect information on various parameters of walking functions is a potential screening methodology for early cognitive decline. Motion capture systems are divided into two categories: one is a conventional optical system with infrared cameras and reflective markers, and the other is an IMU (inertial measurement unit)-based system. It is easier to utilize the IMU-based motion capture system to monitor walking functions in daily environments because it is easy to assemble and measure in any place compared to the conventional optical motion capture system [22] (summarized in Table 1). Ultimately, we aim to establish a database of cognitive and walking functions, enabling us to detect cognitive risks in earlier stages based on walking performances in daily environments collected by the wearable IMU-based motion capture system, including the e-skin MEVA (Fig. 1). The conventional optical motion capture system cannot be utilized to achieve our aim as it requires special facilities and techniques. Even if there are limitations related to the accuracy of tracking data when using the IMU-based system, this system is best suited for our study objectives.
For cognitive assessments, MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination) and MoCA-J (Japanese version of Montreal Cognitive Assessment) are frequently used to detect dementia and MCI. Although these are powerful screening tools for the screening of dementia and MCI, ceiling effects have frequently been reported among healthy participants in these paper-based tests [23,24,25,26]. Thus, web-based cognitive assessment has recently gained attention, as it is more feasible for detecting earlier stages of cognitive decline as compared to paper-based testing [27].
Therefore, we preliminarily examined walking parameters using the e-skin MEVA and cognitive function scores, using web-based assessments (CogEvo, Total Brain Care, Kobe, Japan) in healthy participants (5 women and 6 men; 28–80 years). The CogEvo consisted of five cognitive function domains: spatial recognition, orientation, memory, planning, and attention. These domains were measured by pattern recognition task, confirming the date, flashlight task, maze task, and Trail Making Test, respectively [28, 29]. Resultantly, our preliminary observations, even in a small sample, showed a significant relationship between some cognitive and walking performance modalities in healthy volunteers (see Supplementary Figure S1, Additional file 1). From these results, we establish the study protocol to more comprehensively investigate the relationship between multi-dimensional walking parameters and cognitive function using a custom-based web-application which comprises more cognitive domains than CogEvo (see “Methods” section) and which can be utilized to predict and screen for cognitive function decline. Our study will collect the detailed walking parameters and cognitive assessment scores of a sample of healthy volunteers, aged 20 years or older, to establish more accurate predictive models for cognitive function.
Methods
Study design
The current study, called the Cognitive-Gait Database (CoGait) project, will follow a cross-sectional design aimed at elucidating the relationship between walking and cognitive performance in healthy adults. The sample size was estimated to be 410 persons, calculated using the effect size (f = 0.074) of a similar study [20] under the following conditions: multiple linear model, df = 24, P = 0.01, 1-β= 0.8. The upwardly corrected sample size of 450 people accounts for an attrition rate of 10%. The sample size calculation was performed using G*power [30, 31]. Before submitting this article, we confirmed whether the design was suitable for the STROBE checklist for cross-sectional studies (see Additional file 2). After publication of our study results, we will immediately publish all datasets except personal information in a secured database server.
Study setting and recruitment
Healthy volunteers, older than 20 years and without any neurological or musculoskeletal disorders, will be recruited for the study. The research team consists of a research scientist at Xenoma Inc. (TF), as well as faculty members at the Tokyo University of Technology (YW, TK, and AA), and Dokkyo Medical University (SI). To recruit participants, we will post advertisements about the study through our partners, such as universities (Tokyo University of Technology and Dokkyo Medical University), local governments (Ota-City in Tokyo), local corporate associations, and companies for recruitment testing. To reduce any age-related bias, our advertisement partners will be assigned to collect data from different age generations. For example, younger participants from universities, younger to middle-aged individuals from the local corporate associations, and the elderly from the local governments. Our advertisements will assure prospective participants of their right to withdraw at any time and the financial benefit for participating (approximately 1000 JPY). The study interval is set to 3 years. If we are unable to obtain 450 samples within this time frame, the study will be concluded, and the database will be published as soon as possible. The location of measurement will be gymnasiums and halls in universities and local governments.
Study participants
Inclusion criteria
Healthy participants older than 20 years, without any neurological, musculoskeletal, or psychiatric disorders potentially affecting walking and cognitive functions, will be eligible for participation. Only native Japanese speakers will be recruited. Informed consent will be sought from all participants before they declare their medical histories. In the absence of exclusion criteria (see the “Exclusion criteria” section), participants’ walking, and cognitive performance will be measured (see the “Measures” section).
Exclusion criteria
Participants will be excluded from the study if their medical histories include the disorders or conditions listed in Table 2. Participants with visible abnormalities in walking function (i.e., a mobility function score of <7 on the Functional Independent Measure), as assessed by a skilled physician, physiotherapist, nurse, or research scientist, will also be excluded [32]. Additionally, participants with cognitive problems (i.e., MMSE <24 and MoCA-J ≤25) or self-complaint cognitive dysfunctions assessed by the Geriatric Depression Scale 15 (GDS-15) will be excluded [33].
Measures
General procedure
First, we will obtain written informed consent from all participants. Second, we will obtain their personal information (Table 3), medical histories using the checklists, MMSE and MoCA-J scores, and the questionnaire for subjective memory complaints using GDS-15, in order to exclude participants with pre-diagnostic cognitive disorders (Table 2) [33]. Medical history includes neurological, orthopedic, and psychiatry disorders that potentially affect the walking and cognitive functions (see the “Exclusion criteria” section) [34, 35]. Medical histories will be recorded, and cognitive assessments will be performed by a skilled physician, physiotherapist, nurse, or research scientist face-to-face. Unique personal identities (IDs) will be generated for participants who do not meet the exclusion criteria and printed as QR codes, required during registration for the walking and cognitive assessments. Upon receiving their IDs, participants’ walking will be measured (see the “Gait measurement” section). After a 10-min break, they will participate in the cognitive assessment (see the “Cognitive assessment” section). The walking and cognitive function datasets will be securely stored in online cloud storage. An overview of the study procedures and measurements is presented in Fig. 2.
Gait measurement
We will measure walking parameters using pants, fitted with seven inertial measurement unit (IMU) sensors (e-skin MEVA; Xenoma Inc., Tokyo, Japan), located in the e-textile segments, as shown in Fig. 3A–C. IMU sensors contain triaxial accelerometers and triaxial gyroscopes, enabling the estimation of three-dimensional joint kinematics and global positioning according to a known algorithm [36]. Prior to the development of the current study protocol, we conducted a validity check comparing joint angles across the e-skin MEVA and the conventional optical motion capture system (VICON Nexus ver. 2.1.1, VICON, Oxford, UK). Overall, the systems were comparable (root mean square error: 3.57±1.50°; r = 0.96±0.03, on both sides of the hip, knee, and ankle joints; see Supplementary Figure S2, Additional file 1).
To calibrate the three-dimensional model calculation prior to gait measurement, each participant will be asked to adopt two postures: leaning forward with their hands pressed against a wall (Fig. 3D) and standing upright (Fig. 3E). Next, the participants will be asked to walk in a straight line on a 16-m walkway, including 3-m inlet zones, at the start and end points. The measurements will be conducted under two conditions: fast (maximum speed) and comfortable (self-selected speed). Participants will practice the walking task under each condition several times before the measurements to ensure they understand the requirements of the experimental tasks. In the fast condition, we will instruct participants to walk at their maximum speed, without running or falling. In the comfortable condition, we will instruct them to walk at their regular, comfortable speed.
The measurement datasets will comprise raw IMU sensor signals (acceleration and angular velocity), global positioning of each sensor and anatomical landmark, and joint angles in the pelvis, hip, knee, and ankle (347 parameters in total). The datasets will be automatically uploaded to cloud storage (Fig. 2). The data processing methods are described below (see the “Data analysis” section).
Cognitive assessment
Cognitive assessments will be conducted using the conventional paper-based screening tests: MMSE, MoCA-J, and GDS-15, and a custom-developed web-based software application. Computer-based cognitive assessments cover a wide range of cognitive functions and minimizes floor and ceiling effects [37]. Moreover, such assessments can collect data not only in terms of accuracy of each task, but also in terms of temporal, spatial, and spatiotemporal domains, differentiating them from conventional paper-pencil-based cognitive assessments [38, 39]. Thus, we believe that the combination of conventional paper-based and web-based assessments will assist further studies extended to MCI and MCR.
The software was coded using JavaScript® and runs on a web browser (Safari, Apple, Cupertino, CA). To ensure visual conformity, all tests will be conducted using tablets with the same model number (iPad (8th), Apple, Cupertino, CA). The measure consists of six subtests: (i) simple reaction time (SRT) task, (ii) Go/No-Go task, (iii) Stroop Color–Word Test, (iv) N-back test, (v) Trail Making Test (TMT), and (vi) digit span (DS) test. During the tests, the tablets will be positioned in a landscape orientation and tilted at 20°. Participants’ right index fingers will be placed 2.0 cm behind the tablet. Before any tests, all participants will practice the tasks at least twice, with verbal instructions from the expert staff, using a tablet.
(i) Simple reaction time task
The flow of the SRT task is illustrated in Fig. 4A. Participants will be asked to fix their gaze on the center of the white cross (fixation point), and after the warning signal (1000 Hz, 50 ms), the target signal (red circle) will appear on a black background at random timings (1–3 s after the warning signal). Participants will be asked to press the “はい (Yes)” button with their right index finger when the signal appears [40]. The SRT task comprises 10 trials.
(ii) Go/No-Go task
The flow of the Go/No-Go task is shown in Fig. 4B. The procedure of this task is similar to the SRT task, but the target signal is either a red circle, red triangle, or red cross, on a black background with a 2-s presentation time. The participants will be asked to respond only when the red circle appears on the display. The task consists of 10 trials each, for the “Go” and “No-Go” paradigms [41]. The Go/No-Go ratio was determined such that the reaction time was prolonged compared to SRT in healthy volunteers (n = 4; M = 38.5 years, SD = 12.2; Supplementary Figure S3A, Additional file 1).
(iii) Stroop Color–Word Test
The Stroop Color–Word Test was translated from a previous study [42] and converted into a digitalized test, using a tablet. The color words are displayed as target signals after the warning signal. The target signals are the Japanese words (kanji) [RED], [BLUE], and [GREEN], with the font color set to one of these colors, on a black background, with a 2-s presentation time (Fig. 4C). Participants will be asked to respond only when any word in a red font appears (Go trial). The Go trial consists of congruent (color and word match) and incongruent (color and word mismatch) conditions [43]. The ratio between congruent and incongruent groups is 1:1 [43], and the task consists of 10 trials each, for the Go and No-Go paradigms. In addition to the Go/No-Go task, the reaction time was prolonged compared to SRT in healthy volunteers (n = 4; M = 38.5 years, SD =12.2; Supplementary Figure S3A, Additional file 1).
(iv) N-back task
The N-back task is a major approach used for assessing working memory capacity [44, 45]. Single-digit numbers will be displayed on the tablet as target signals (Fig. 5A), with a presentation time of 2 s for each target signal.
In the one-back condition, the participants will be asked to respond only when the target signal is the same as the last number displayed (congruent condition). In the two- and three-back conditions, they should respond only when the target signals are the same as the second-to-last and third-to-last numbers displayed, respectively. The frequency of the congruent condition will be set to 44% of the target signals, and the task consists of 15 trials for each n-back condition. In this condition, reaction times in healthy volunteers (n = 5; M = 34.4 years, SD = 4.98) were prolonged in the N = 1 and 2, compared to N =1 (Supplementary Figures S3B and C, Additional file 1).
(v) Trail making test
We adapted the Japanese version of the paper-based TMT, so that the data could be uploaded to our cloud storage [46]. The TMT consists of the TMT-A and TMT-B. The TMT-A contains 25 circled numbers, ranging from 1 to 25; participants will be asked to tap the circled numbers in order, from 1 to 25 (Fig. 5B). The TMT-B contains 13 circled numbers, ranging from 1 to 13, and 12 circled Japanese kana letters; participants will be asked to tap the numbers and letters following the rule 1-[あ]-2-[い]-3-[う]…[し]-13, corresponding to the original alphabet version of TMT-B (Fig. 5B) [46, 47]. In our adapted TMT, the participants will not need to draw a line on the screen to prevent misrecognition of tapping on non-target symbols. From preliminary experiments, the process time for the TMT-B in healthy volunteers (n = 4; M = 30.3 years, SD = 4.03) was markedly prolonged compared to that for the TMT-A, which was similar to previous results (Supplementary Figure S3D, Additional file 1) [48].
(vi) Digit span test
The DS test is a well-established measure of working memory capacity [49, 50]. The digit indicator and numeric keypad used in the DS are shown in Fig. 5C. Numerical digits are presented individually and sequentially, and the participants will be asked to remember the sequence of the presentation. Next, the participants will be asked to recall the sequence, using the numeric keypad. The DS test will be conducted under forward and backward conditions. The participants will be required to recall the sequences in forward or backward direction, depending on the task condition; sequence length ranges, from two to nine numerical digits, in both tasks. Participants will be required to perform the test under both conditions, and the trials will be repeated three times for each sequence length. When participants record three mistakes in the same sequence, the DS test will be completed. In our preliminary experiment with healthy volunteers (n = 4; 29.8±2.68 years), the matching rate for the backward condition was relatively low for longer target number sequences (Supplementary Figure S3E, Additional file 1).
Data analysis
Gait analysis
From the gait measurement datasets, we will calculate the general walking parameters, such as stride length and minimum toe clearance, using the built-in software (e-skin LETS WALK, Xenoma Inc., Tokyo, Japan). The general parameters are presented in Table 4. The sweeps of raw signals, such as IMU data, joint angle, and global positioning of each sensor or anatomical landmark, will be averaged with the time normalized by the percentage of the stride cycle. When the stride cycles cannot be defined because of poor data quality, the dataset will be excluded.
Psychological analysis
A cognitive performance is evaluated by reaction time, process time, and task accuracy. The reaction time will be calculated for the SRT, Go/No-Go task, Stroop Color–Words Test, and N-back test. The reaction time is defined as the interval between the onset of the target signal and the participant’s response. The process time is defined as the interval between the task onset and completion in the TMT and DS test. Furthermore, we will compare the cognitive performance with the scores of the conventional paper-based assessments (MMSE and MoCA-J) using multiple linear regression analysis in order to confirm the validity of our custom-made application.
Statistical analysis
In this study, we will establish statistical models for predicting cognitive functions based on walking characteristics in healthy participants. At first, we will exclude the data obtained by MCR participants for the statistical analysis. MCR will be defined using the algorithm by Marquez et al. [33]. According to this study, we defined MCR syndrome as slower comfortable walking speed (-1SD below the mean of whole dataset) and subjective memory complaint as detected by the GDS-15. After the exclusion of the data obtained from MCR participants, we will perform the statistical analyses using the following processes.
The dependent variables are reaction time, process time, and task accuracy, and the independent variables are walking parameters and averaged signal traces, in both the fast and comfortable conditions. Prior to the substitution of the independent variables in the statistical model, we will sift the variables, to prevent problems related to multi-covariance [51].
The walking parameters will be reduced to less than 25 dimensions, by principal component analysis (PCA), as needed. The statistical model will be used for multiple linear regression, with and without the random sample consensus (RANSAC) algorithm. The accuracy of the statistical model will be evaluated using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) [52]. All statistical analyses will be performed using the Python script, with the scikit-learn library.
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this will be the first study to use a three-dimensional motion capture system to reveal the relationship between walking and cognitive functions in healthy adults. This database will be essential for developing a system that detects early cognitive risks based on walking performances in a daily environment. As described in the “Introduction” section, the conventional optical motion capture system requires more than three cameras and several reflective markers, which are located in laboratory environments and not daily situations. Thus, the IMU-based motion capture system is more suitable for the objectives of this study as it can be embedded into wearable devices, such as the e-skin MEVA in this study.
From a methodological perspective, the accuracy of the algorithm for three-dimensional bone modeling in the e-skin MEVA has already been confirmed in a previous study and was also supported by the findings of our preliminary experiment, comparing it with conventional motion capture systems (see Supplementary Figure S2, Additional file 1) [36]. In fact, the e-skin MEVA and LETS WALK have already been applied in clinical fields to detect abnormal walking patterns owing to spinal cord injury (Higashibaba and Irie, in submission). Thus, we believe that the accuracy of three-dimensional motion capture, using the e-skin MEVA, is adequate for this study. Furthermore, the raw IMU data during walking will be published with our study database, which would enable users and developers of other IMU devices to use the database for their own healthcare applications. However, it is important to assess the validity of the IMU-based system compared to the conventional optical motion capture system to improve the comparability of the database. Thus, we also plan future validity studies of the e-skin MEVA in multiple locations and to publish those datasets simultaneously as the database from this study.
For cognitive assessments, we used both the custom-made web-based- and paper-based cognitive assessments in this protocol. As described in the “Introduction” section, conventional paper-based testing methods, such as MMSE and MoCA, are not suitable for this study because of their ceiling effects in healthy participants [23,24,25,26]. Moreover, web-based cognitive assessment has recently gained attention because of its feasibility in detecting early stage cognitive decline compared to paper-based testing [27].
In particular, our custom-made application could better assess higher dimensional cognitive performances, including the temporal, spatial, and spatio-temporal domains, than conventional applications. Additionally, we will obtain scores of the conventional paper-based assessments (MMSE, MoCA-J, and GDS-15). These scores will be used for the screening of pre-diagnostic dementia, MCI, and MCR and for comparison with variables obtained by the web-based application for validity testing, which will assist further studies aiming to detect MCI and MCR.
The associations between walking and cognitive functions have been well described in studies on cognitive interference in walking. Killeen et al. reported that minimum toe clearance decreased while a Stroop Color-Word test was administered under experimental conditions [53]. In addition, there have been several reports about such interferences, assessed using the well-known dual task paradigm [54,55,56], considered to be compensatory mechanisms and/or overlapping functional localization [57]. These common neural mechanisms in walking and cognition are related to changes in walking characteristics that precede cognitive decline. However, the relationship between these interferences in dual-task paradigms and natural walking parameters without any cognitive loading is unclear. Further studies are required to elucidate this relationship.
This study has several limitations. First, although we checked the reliability of the e-skin MEVA compared to the conventional motion capture system, there are several errors in the three-dimensional model calculation because the MEVA algorithm calculates a three-dimensional model based on gender and body height, not including the length of each segment [36]. Moreover, the accuracy of the IMU-based motion capture system relies on the pitch (cadence) of walking [58]. Second, our cognitive assessment tools have not been compared to conventional tools, such as the MMSE and MoCA, which might decrease the reliability of the overall experiment from a methodological perspective.
For gait measurement, we posit that this study focuses on revealing the relationship between walking and cognitive functions in healthy participants. We also aim to utilize the database to detect early cognitive risks for MCI and MCR patients from walking in a daily environment, which will be realized by further studies. Thus, the IMU-based motion capture system is more suitable for gait measurement in this study than the conventional optical motion capture system from the perspective of the daily monitoring, even though the accuracy of the IMU-based system is inferior to the conventional optical system [59]. Additionally, it is important that the comparison between the IMU-based (e-skin MEVA) and conventional optical systems is published on open repositories in order to assure compatibility with other measurement systems. In this study, we will publish the dataset of this comparison simultaneous to the publication of the whole database, which will improve the utility of the database so that it will be available for any device and environment [60].
For cognitive assessments, our preliminary experiments indicated a significant relationship between parts of walking and cognitive parameters (see Supplementary Figure S3, Additional file 1) [42, 48, 60, 61]. Additionally, our web-based applications were created based on a well-established psychological paradigm that is sensitive to early cognitive decline, even in healthy participants, in whom functional localizations and relationship between cognitive impairments have already been detected [62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70]. Thus, we believe that our cognitive assessment application has adequate reliability to detect the relationships between the walking and cognitive functions [42, 48, 62, 63]. Additionally, we will compare the results of the paper-based and our custom-made application in this study. Thus, it will allow us to observe the compatibility, superiority, and inferiority of our application comparing with conventional paper-based tests.
For clinical implications, the database developed in this study could provide standard values for both walking and cognitive functions, which would support the evaluation of psychomotor function, including the MCR syndrome. Our research team also plans to conduct a cohort study for participants, to define the risks of MCR and MCI. This CoGait project database will be developed for use as a worldwide platform, for cross-sectional and longitudinal studies on cognition, walking, and frailty, as well as other studies in the field of geriatrics.
Availability of data and materials
After publication, the database of walking and cognitive parameters developed in this study will be published as a public repository. Access permissions will be managed by the Tokyo University of Technology and Xenoma Inc.
Abbreviations
- AIC:
-
Akaike’s information criterion
- DS:
-
Digit span
- GDS-15:
-
Geriatric Depression Scale 15
- IMU:
-
Inertial measurement unit
- MCI:
-
Mild cognitive impairment
- MCR:
-
Motoric cognitive risk
- MMSE:
-
Mini-Mental State Examination
- MoCA:
-
Montreal cognitive assessment
- RANSAC:
-
Random sample consensus
- SRT:
-
Simple reaction time
- TMT:
-
Trail Making Test
References
Verghese J. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: next steps. Eur J Neurol. 2021;28:2467–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/ene.14949.
Camicioli R, Howieson D, Oken B, Sexton G, Kaye J. Motor slowing precedes cognitive impairment in the oldest old. Neurology. 1998;50:1496–8. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.50.5.1496.
Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med J. 2013;68:412–8.
Verghese J, Annweiler C, Ayers E, Barzilai N, Beauchet O, Bennett DA, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: multicountry prevalence and dementia risk. Neurology. 2014;83:718–26.
Jedrziewski MK, Lee VM-Y, Trojanowski JQ. Physical activity and cognitive health. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3:98–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.009.
Jedrziewski MK, Ewbank DC, Wang H, Trojanowski JQ. Exercise and cognition: results from the National Long Term Care Survey. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:448–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2010.02.004.
Jedrziewski MK, Ewbank DC, Wang H, Trojanowski JQ. The impact of exercise, cognitive activities, and socialization on cognitive function: results from the National Long-Term Care Survey. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2014;29:372–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317513518646.
Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, Bennett D, Doody R, Ferris S, et al. Vitamin E and Donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. NEJM. 2005;352:2379–88. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa050151.
Rosenberg A, Mangialasche F, Ngandu T, Solomon A, Kivipelto M. Brief reports multidomain interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and dementia: from FINGER to World-Wide FINGERS. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2020;7:29–36. https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2019.41.
Doi T, Mbbs JV, Makizako H, Tsutsumimoto K, Phd H, Nakakubo S, et al. Effects of cognitive leisure activity on cognition in mild cognitive impairment: results of a randomized controlled trial. JAMDA. 2017;18:686–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2017.02.013.
Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med. 2006;144:73. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004.
Luchsinger JA, Tang M-X, Shea S, Mayeux R. Caloric intake and the risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:1258. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.59.8.1258.
Luchsinger JA, Tang M-X, Siddiqui M, Shea S, Mayeux R. Alcohol intake and risk of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:540–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52159.x.
Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet, Alzheimer disease, and vascular mediation. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1709. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.63.12.noc60109.
Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang M-X, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:912–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20854.
Verghese J, Ayers E, Barzilai N, Bennett DA, Buchman AS, Holtzer R, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: multicenter incidence study. Neurology. 2014;83:2278–84. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001084.
Norwitz NG, Saif N, Ariza IE, Isaacson RS. Precision nutrition for Alzheimer’s prevention in ApoE4 carriers. Nutrients. 2021;13:1362. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13041362.
Sabia S, Fayosse A, Dumurgier J, van Hees VT, Paquet C, Sommerlad A, et al. Association of sleep duration in middle and old age with incidence of dementia. Nat Commun. 2021;12:2289.
Matthews FE, Arthur A, Barnes LE, Bond J, Jagger C, Robinson L, et al. A two-decade comparison of prevalence of dementia in individuals aged 65 years and older from three geographical areas of England: results of the cognitive function and ageing study I and II. Lancet. 2013;382:1405–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61570-6.
Jongki C, Jinse P, Byung-Inn L, Kyoung S, Sunmi Y, Hyoeun K, et al. The correlation between cognition screening scores and Gait status from three-dimensional Gait analysis. J Clin Neurol. 2019;15:152–8. https://doi.org/10.3988/jcn.2019.15.2.152.
Knapstad MK, Steihaug OM, Aaslund MK, Nakling A, Naterstad IF, Fladby T, et al. Reduced walking speed in subjective and mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2019;42:E122–8. https://doi.org/10.1519/JPT.0000000000000157qwqqw.
Maruyama T, Tada M, Toda H. Riding motion capture system using inertial measurement units with contact constraints. Int J Autom Technol. 2019;13:506–16. https://doi.org/10.20965/ijat.2019.p0506.
Breton A, Casey D, Arnaoutoglou NA. Cognitive tests for the detection of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the prodromal stage of dementia: meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy studies. Int J of Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34:233–42. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5016.
Kaufer DI, Williams CS, Braaten AJ, Gill K, Zimmerman S, Sloane PD. Cognitive screening for dementia and mild cognitive impairment in assisted living: comparison of 3 tests. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2008;9:586–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2008.05.006.
Mitchell AJ. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43:411–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.04.014.
Ruan Q, Zhang W, Ruan J, Chen J, Yu Z. Clinical and objective cognitive measures for the diagnosis of cognitive frailty subtypes: a comparative study. Front Psychol. 2021;12:603974. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.603974.
Devenney KE, Sanders ML, Lawlor B, Olde Rikkert MGM, Schneider S. The effects of an extensive exercise programme on the progression of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI): study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17:75. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-017-0457-9.
CogEvoと認知機能 – CogEvo [Internet]. Available from: https://cog-evo.jp/cognitive-function/. Cited 2022 Jun 6.
Ichii S, Nakamura T, Kawarabayashi T, Takatama M, Ohgami T, Ihara K, et al. CogEvo, a cognitive function balancer, is a sensitive and easy psychiatric test battery for age-related cognitive decline. Geriatr GerontolInt. 2020;20:248–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.13847.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.
Granger CV, Hamilton BB, Linacre JM, Heinemann AW, Wright BD. Performance profiles of the functional independence measure. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 1993;72:84–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002060-199304000-00005.
Marquez I, Garcia-Cifuentes E, Velandia FR, Iragorri A, Saavedra AM, Borda MG, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: prevalence and cognitive performance. A cross-sectional study. Lancet Regional Health Am. 2022;8:100162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lana.2021.100162.
Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390:2673–734. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6.
Perry J, Burnfield J. Gait analysis: normal and pathological function. 2nd ed. Thorofare: Slack Inc; 2010.
Teufl W, Miezal M, Taetz B, Fröhlich M, Bleser G. Validity, test-retest reliability and long-term stability of magnetometer free inertial sensor based 3D joint kinematics. Sensors. 2018;18:1980. https://doi.org/10.3390/s18071980.
Wild K, Howieson D, Webbe F, Seelye A, Kaye J. Status of computerized cognitive testing in aging: a systematic review. Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4:428–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.07.003.
Makdissi M. Computerised cognitive assessment of concussed Australian rules footballers. Br J Sports Med. 2001;35:354–60. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.35.5.354.
Cysique LAJ, Maruff P, Darby D, Brew BJ. The assessment of cognitive function in advanced HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia complex using a new computerised cognitive test battery. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21:185–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.011.
Lasaponara S, Glicksohn J, Mauro F, Ben-Soussan TD. Contingent negative variation and P3 modulations following mindful movement training. Prog Brain Res. 2019;244:101–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.017.
Baijot S, Cevallos C, Zarka D, Leroy A, Slama H, Colin C, et al. EEG Dynamics of a Go/Nogo task in children with ADHD. Brain Sci. 2017;7:167. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7120167.
Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651.
Shen C, Jiang Q, Luo Y, Long J, Tai X, Liu S. Stroop interference in children with developmental dyslexia. Medicine. 2021;100:e26464. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000026464.
Craik FIM. Age differences in recognition memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 1971;23:316–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640746908401827.
Grässler B, Herold F, Dordevic M, Gujar TA, Darius S, Böckelmann I, et al. Multimodal measurement approach to identify individuals with mild cognitive impairment: study protocol for a cross-sectional trial. BMJ Open. 2021;11:46879. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-046879.
Toyokura M, Sawatari M, Nishimura Y, Ishida A. Nondominant hand performance of the Japanese trail making test and its mirror version. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84:691–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0003-9993(02)04904-3.
David Wechsler. WMS-III: Wechsler Memory scale administration and scoring manual. Psychological Corporation; place of pub: 1997.
Tombaugh T. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8.
Cherry B. Better preservation of memory span relative to supraspan immediate recall in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:846–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00173-7.
Belleville S, Peretz I, Malenfant D. Examination of the working memory components in normal aging and in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:195–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00097-6.
Kumar TK. Multicollinearity in regression analysis. Rev Econ Stat. 1975;57:365–6.
Akaike H. Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: 2nd International Symposium on Information Theory: Akademiai Kiado; 1973. p. 267–81.
Killeen T, Easthope CS, Demkó L, Filli L, Lőrincz L, Linnebank M, et al. Minimum toe clearance: probing the neural control of locomotion. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1922. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02189-y.
Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Yogev G, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov Disord. 2006;21:950–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20848.
Doi T, Makizako H, Shimada H, Park H, Tsutsumimoto K, Uemura K, et al. Brain activation during dual-task walking and executive function among older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a fNIRS study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2013;25:539–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-013-0119-5.
Teo WP, Rantalainen T, Nuzum N, Valente L, Macpherson H. Altered prefrontal cortex responses in older adults with subjective memory complaints and dementia during dual-task gait: an fNIRS study. Eur J Neurosci. 2021;53:1324–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14989.
Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA. Neurocognitive Aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17:177–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x.
Majumder S, Deen MJ. Wearable IMU-based system for real-time monitoring of lower-limb joints. IEEE Sensors J. 2021;21:8267–75. https://doi.org/10.1109/JSEN.2020.3044800.
Poitras I, Dupuis F, Bielmann M, Campeau-Lecours A, Mercier C, Bouyer L, et al. Validity and reliability of wearable Sensors for joint angle estimation: a systematic review. Sensors. 2019;19:1555. https://doi.org/10.3390/s19071555.
Miller JO, Low K. Motor processes in simple, go/no-go, and choice reaction time tasks: a psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27:266–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.266.
Cho YS, Jang SH, Cho JS, Kim MJ, Lee HD, Lee SY, et al. Evaluation of validity and reliability of inertial measurement unit-based gait analysis systems. Ann Rehabil Med. 2018;42:872–83. https://doi.org/10.5535/arm.2018.42.6.872.
Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci. 2013;34:749–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-012-1130-x.
Tsai C-K, Kao T-W, Lee J-T, Wang C-C, Chou C-H, Liang C-S, et al. Global-cognitive health metrics: a novel approach for assessing cognition impairment in adult population. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0197691.
Kumar A, Migliore S, Sollievo C, Sofferenza D, Kandimalla R, Guarino A, et al. Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: a systematic review. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;1:437. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437.
O’Callaghan C, Naismith SL, Hodges JR, Lewis SJG, Hornberger M. Fronto-striatal atrophy correlates of inhibitory dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease versus behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2013;49:1833–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.003.
Specka M, Weimar C, Stang A, Jöckel K-H, Scherbaum N, Hoffmann SS, et al. Trail making test normative data for the German older population. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2021;00:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab027.
Jäncke L, Gerson Unschuld P, Faraza S, Waldenmaier J, Dyrba M, Wolf D, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal functional connectivity predicts working memory training gains. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021;13:592261. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.592261.
Menon RS, Luknowsky DC, Gati JS. Mental chronometry using latency-resolved functional MRI. Neurobiology. 1998;95:10902–7. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.18.10902.
Motes MA, Rao NK, Shokri-Kojori E, Chiang H-S, Kraut MA, Hart J. Trial-level regressor modulation for functional magnetic resonance imaging designs requiring strict periodicity of stimulus presentations: illustrated using a go/no-go task. Magn Reason Insights. 2017;10:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1177/1178623X17746693.
Nordlund A, Rolstad S, Klang O, Edman A, Hansen S, Wallin A. Two-year outcome of MCI subtypes and aetiologies in the Goteborg MCI study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81:541–6. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.171066.
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to acknowledge Dr. Amimori (CEO of Xenoma Inc.), Dr. Amano (Chief Scientist at Xenoma Inc.), Mrs. Amimori (Engineering Specialist at Xenoma Inc.), and Mr. Hirasawa, Ms. Tatsuyama (Chief Designer at Xenoma Inc.), Mr. Ueda (Chief Engineers at Xenoma Inc.), and Dr. Sakata (Professor of Dokkyo Medical University) for their help with information technology and management.
Funding
This work was supported by the Strategic International Research Cooperative Program (SICORP) and Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST) for the “Continuous health status monitoring of elderly people, using flexible skin patch sensors” (TF and SI; Grant no. JPMJSC1812), and Xenoma Inc. (SI and YW).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
TF, SI, YW, TK, and AA contributed to the study design. TF, SI, and YW were involved in the preliminary experiments of gait measurements. TF, SI, and AA were involved in the preliminary cognitive assessments. All authors critically revised the report, commented on drafts of the manuscript, and approved the final report. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This research protocol was approved by the local ethics committee of the Tokyo University of Technology (approval no. E20HS-038) and Dokkyo Medical University (approval no. 2021-011), in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Prior to conducting our study, we will obtain participants’ written informed consent and assure them of their right to withdraw their consent at any time, for any reason.
Consent for publication
At registration, we will obtain participants’ consent for publication. When a participant withdraws consent, we will exclude their data from the published data.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests. YW received research funding from Xenoma Inc., associated with the Cooperative Research and Development Agreement. SI received research funding from Xenoma Inc., as a donation to Dokkyo Medical University.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Additional file 1:
Preliminary results. We performed preliminary experiments before developing this study protocol. Figure S1. depicts the result of a pilot observation of the relationship between the walking and cognitive parameters in a small population. Figure S2. shows the result of comparisons of joint angles between MEVA and VICON (conventional motion capture system). Figure S3. presents the results of preliminary experiments of cognitive performance using a custom-made web-based application. Figure S4. shows the rule for the Japanese kana letters in the TMT-B test.
Additional file 2.
STROBE checklist. The STROBE checklist for cross-sectional studies. Each cell in the line numbers column indicates the line numbers within the manuscript where each item of the checklist is located.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Fukuoka, T., Irie, S., Watanabe, Y. et al. The relationship between spatiotemporal gait parameters and cognitive function in healthy adults: protocol for a cross-sectional study. Pilot Feasibility Stud 8, 154 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-022-01122-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-022-01122-z